- [ Polynon ]
- [ Whitepaper ]
The Polynon: A Geometry of Consciousness
- [ Abstract ]
This work presents a geometric framework for cognition, centered on a conceptual polytope, the Polynon, to explore consciousness and its relationship with physical reality. Adopting an analytic idealist view, the Polynon posits consciousness as the foundational element of existence, preceding physical phenomena. A polynonial mechanism is introduced, offering a geometric and ontological interpretation of consciousness, where both reality and the observer unfold as holographic projections. This framework maps consciousness by analyzing epistemological interactions among phenomena (sensory experience), phantasiai (internal representations), and noumena (underlying reality). Cognitive gravity and gradients provide metrics for cognitive dimensions, while a continuum of perceptual dimensions is proposed, with the wavefunction in superposition linking the observer’s cognition to hidden physical realities. This model connects cognition to reality’s structure, proposing a new mechanism for the observer’s measurement.
- [ Keywords ]
Consciousness, Hologram, Cognitive Gravity, Noumena, Wavefunction, Observer, Reality
- [ Cite ]
- [ Roibu, T. (2025) The Polynon: A Geometry of Consciousness ]
1 Introduction
Inspired by Gestalt psychology, geometric perception and cognition reveal universal laws governing cognitive processes, extending beyond the human condition and highlighting the limitations of human cognition. Recognizing these limitations, we accept that our understanding of reality is shaped by both cognitive capabilities and constraints, suggesting our models reflect structured interpretations rather than reality itself.
In this view, the polynon’s geometric representations serve as a form of epistemic ontology, indicating that our cognitive models reflect not the world itself but our structured interpretation of it.
This emphasizes that our interaction with reality is mediated by cognitive structures, including perception, imagination, and noumenal factors, suggesting a complex engagement with the world beyond mere reflection. Just as thoughts shape internal experiences into symbols and perceptual dimensions, the polynon provides a geometric framework for cognitive processes. This approach unfolds across infinite cognitive dimensions, utilizing phenomena (p+), phantasiai (p-), and noumena (n).
Geometry is not a cultural invention, but a universal mental construction.
These forms represent the course of development of human consciousness from the animal basis, the pure sense-consciousness, to the spiritual or divine consciousness; both which extremes are not man — the one underlying, the other transcending the limits of human evolution.
2 The polynon
An entity is a self-contained existence that has a distinct presence and objective or conceptual reality. In Kantian philosophy a noumenon is a posited object or an event that exists independently of human sense and/or perception. The phenomenal (p+) is the dimension of physical. The phantasiai (p-) is the dimension of the epiphenomenal, the mental. The noumenal is the fundamental existence. Points (p) are representations for perceptions, individual measurements, and the wavefunction collapse. An event (e) occurs when an observer (O) is measuring the collapse of the wavefunction. Non-events are both negative and positive noumena (n). All positive noumena (n+) in a polynon are equal and identical. All are projected into cognitive dimensions.
3 Phantasiai et Phenomena
4 The things in themselves
It is impossible to separate phenomenon from noumenon, knowledge of both being essential for a complete understanding of reality.
5 Noumena, Non-Nomic, Non-event, Novent
6 Psychophysical monism
If something out of nothing can appear, then nothing holds all that can dissapear.
While we cannot have empirical knowledge of the noumenal self, we can have practical knowledge of it through the mechanisms of reflection and self-reflection.
Eternity isn’t some later time. Eternity isn’t even a long time. Eternity has nothing to do with time. Eternity is that dimension of here and now that all thinking in temporal terms cuts off…. the experience of eternity right here and now, in all things, … is the function of life. (Joseph Campbell, The Power of Myth with Bill Moyers, PBS, 1988)
In the polynon, phenomena (p+), phantasiai (p-), and noumena (n) are entwined within the cognitive dimensions of an observer O(n), challenging the notion that phenomena alone constitute fundamental reality. This perspective highlights the logical fallacy in physicalist assertions that prioritize the measurable over the experiential, denying the unknowable. Campbell’s concept of eternity transcends temporal limitations, existing within the “here and now” as a dimension of consciousness integral to the observer’s self-reflective function.
This eternal aspect of consciousness, a timeless monadic essence, is described as the perceptual continuum of f=0, signifying pure, timeless potential. The act of observation, akin to plucking this string, transforms potential into actual phenomena, reflecting consciousness’s vibrations through reality, wavefunctions, and the infinite possibilities within it. The polynon’s treatment of consciousness as Conscia Noumena, interwoven with psychophysical monism and the perceptual monad, posits consciousness as the primordial essence from which observable phenomena emerge.
This paradigm shifts the focus from attempting to quantify and measure to an understanding thorugh embodying the depth and richness of consciousness, by transcending the traditional dichotomy and perceptual fragmentation between the measurable and the experiential.

7 The Lens-prism interpretation of the wavefunction
Incorporating n as a ‘revealed variable’ aligns with Bohm’s interpretation of quantum mechanics, suggesting that understanding n could yield deterministic predictions within a landscape of infinite potential and probability. Bohm’s theory posits that hidden variables, when unveiled, reveal a deterministic layer beneath quantum randomness. Similarly, if the noumenal variable
n is accurately identified, it could expose a deterministic structure underlying the probabilistic nature of quantum events, reconciling indeterminacy with fundamental order. The structure of the lens in the polynon suggests that noumena “closer” to the observer’s perceptual system are more likely to collapse into phenomena, with this “closeness” not bound by time or space.
Viewing n as part of Bohm’s “implicate order” introduces it as an inherent quantum property that becomes explicit upon measurement. Bohm’s concept posits that all elements in the universe are interconnected within a deeper, non-local reality, where n serves as a hidden, intrinsic quality that observation brings to light.
Thus, while perceptual processes in the polynon appear deterministic, governed by initial conditions and intrinsic structure, they also contain an indeterministic nature due to the fundamentally unknowable aspects of noumena. Cognitive outcomes, though traceable to specific conditions, embody a dual essence—deterministic yet indeterministic—highlighting the balance between order and chaos in consciousness and cognition.
The collapse of the wavefunction becomes an iterative process of phenomenal observations (p+) and (p-), influenced by the observer’s properties and the medium through which observation occurs. This process illustrates how the sum of wavefunctions, serving as a diffractive lens, transmutes the noumenal into a spectrum of observable phenomena and phantasiai (p1(On), p2(On), …, pn(On)), each reflecting the same underlying noumenal reality.
8 The Observer
The Observer, in this construct, is envisioned as a holographic projection sitting at the core of the noumenal lens, emerging from the focus and diffraction of noumenal probabilities within the noumenal lens. This conceptualization likens the simplest form of an observer to a monad, a single phenomenal construct surrounded by its noumenal subtance, illustrating an infinite array of perceptual lenses in superposition, with the noumenal at their margins and the observer positioned at the perceptual core. The intersection of noumenal probabilities with the observer’s perceptual lens acts as a focusing mechanism, directing the collapse of the noumena into the observer’s cognitive field.
This process of focusing and diffraction amplifies the reflexivity between the observer and the observed, enhancing the observer’s ability to perceive and interact with the noumenal dimension, only to reveal that there is no real distinction between them. As the number of noumenal collapses increases, the complexity and breadth of the observer’s perceptual dimension diminishes, resulting in a progressive refinement and narrowing of perceptual prowess.
The observer’s cognitive framework synthesizes a multidimensional mechanism, much like a holographic parabolic mirror capturing and projecting a three-dimensional image from scattered light sources. The lens acts as a diffractive medium for noumena while also serving as a focusing function, adjusting noumenal factors to create an observable construct, where each collapse contributes to a richer, more detailed representation of a phenomenal reality.
Yet, this narrowing of perceptual prowess does not signify a loss but rather the embodied purpose of self-reflection of consciousness: while the breadth of perception diminishes, the precision and clarity of the observer’s interaction with phenomenal reality deepen. And, it is through the recursive focus between noumena and phenomena that the observer discovers the underlying unity of existence, where the distinctions between the observer and the observed dissolve, and reality itself emerges as a seamless continuum of consciousness and form.
9 Cognitive gravity and gradient
9.1 The noumenal gradient
9.2 The cognitive gradient
10 The hologram
The polynon is abstract and elusive, residing beyond human imagination yet within its grasp. No heavier than a fleeting idea, but far too heavy to be understood. A palindrome for language and thought, mirroring itself in perpetual paradox. Beginning the same way it ends.
11 Conclusion
- [ References]
- Agrawal, P., Stansbury, D., Malik, J., Gallant J. (2014). Pixels to Voxels: Modeling Visual Representation in the Human Brain. ArXiv. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1407.5104
- Al-Fedaghi, S. (2023). Ontology for Conceptual Modeling: Reality of What Thinging Machines Talk About, e.g., Information. ArXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2308.09483
- Alloa, E. (2018). El poder de visualizar. La ‘phantasia’ según Aristóteles. Anuario Filosófico. https://doi.org/10.15581/009.51.2.243-274
- Berkeley, G.; Turbayne, Colin Murray (1957). A Treatise Concerning the Principles of Human Knowledge. Forgotten Books. ISBN 978-1-60506-970-8.
- Bergson, H. (1896). Matter and Memory. Zone Books, New York.
- Bernardi S., Benna, M.K., Rigotti M., Munuera J., Fusi, S., Salzman D. (2020). The Geometry of Abstraction in the Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2020.09.031
- Beuzekom, A. D., Van Gisbergen, J. A. M. (2000). Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates. Journal of Neuropsychology, Vol. 84, pp 11-27. https://doi.org/10.1152/JN.2000.84.1.11
- Bohm, D. (1980). Wholeness and the Implicate Order (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203995150
- Borelli, C., Berneburg, M. (2009). Beauty lies in the eye of the beholder’? Aspects of beauty and attractiveness. Journal der Deutschen Dermatologischen Gesellschaft. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1610-0387.2009.07318_supp.x
- Cadwallader, M. (1981). Towards a cognitive gravity model: The case of consumer spatial behaviour. Regional Studies, 15:4, 275-284. DOI: 10.1080/09595238100185281
- Carr, B. (2021). Making space and time for consciousness in physics. In Paul Dennison (ed.), Perspectives on Consciousness. New York: Nova Science. pp. 319-350.
- Caves C. M., Fuchs C. A.,Schack R., “Quantum Probabilities as Bayesian Probabilities,” Phys. Rev. 10.1103/PhysRevA.65.022305
- Chalmers, D.J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. Oxford University Press.
- Chalmers, D.J. (2003). Consciousness and its place in nature. S. Stich, T. Warfield (Eds.), Blackwell Guide to Philosophy of Mind, Blackwell
- Chalmers, D. J., & McQueen, K. J. (in press). Consciousness and the Collapse of the Wave Function. In S. Gao (Ed.), Consciousness and Quantum Mechanics. Oxford University Press.
- Chen, E.K. (2020). Fundamental Nomic Vagueness. Arxiv. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2006.05298
- Citti, G., Sarti A.(2019). Neurogeometry of perception: isotropic and anisotropic aspects. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1906.03495
- Cohen, M.M. (1992). Perception and Action in Altered Gravity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25221.x
- Cohoe, C. (2016). When and Why Understanding Needs Phantasmata: A Moderate Interpretation of Aristotle’s De Memoria and De Anima on the Role of Images in Intellectual Activities. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685284-12341311
- Cook, L.S. (1887). Geometrical Psychology, or, The Science of Representation: an Abstract of the Theories and Diagrams of B. W. Betts. G. Redway, London.
- De Cicco, J. (2014). Non-Euclidean geometry. https://doi.org/10.1036/1097-8542.455300
- Delle Monache, D., Indovina, I., Zago, M., Daprati, E., Lacquaniti, F., Bosco, G. (2021). Watching the Effects of Gravity. Vestibular Cortex and the Neural Representation of ‘Visual’ Gravity. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2021.793634
- Doursat, R. (2013). Bridging the Mind-Brain Gap by Morphogenetic ‘Neuron Flocking’: The Dynamic Self-Organization of Neural Activity into Mental Shapes. AAAI Fall Symposia. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:53306293
- Du, C., Du, C., He, H. (2017). Sharing Deep Generative Representation for Perceived Image Reconstruction from Human Brain Activity. IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Network. https://doi.org/10.1109/IJCNN.2017.7965968
- Du, C., Du, C., Huang, L., He, H. (2019). Reconstructing Perceived Images From Human Brain Activities With Bayesian Deep Multiview Learning. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks and Learning Systems. https://doi.org/10.1109/TNNLS.2018.2882456
- Emundts, D. (2010). The Refutation of Idealism and the Distinction between Phenomena and Noumena. https://doi.org/10.1017/CCOL9780521883863.008
- Esfeld, M. (2020). “Thing’ and ‘non-thing’ ontologies. The Routledge Handbook of Metametaphysics, 1st Edition. ISBN 9781315112596
- Fabri, E. (2021). Ontology.” Research Methods in the Social Sciences: An A-Z of key concepts. https://doi.org/10.1093/hepl/9780198850298.003.0046
- Facco, E., Pederzoli, L., Tressoldi, P.E. (2020). Non-Ordinary Mental Expressions (NOMEs): clues on the nature of the human mind. Social Science Research Network. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ac2n7
- Fichte, J. G. (1889). The Science of Knowledge. London: Trubner & Co., Ludgate Hill.
- Buckminster, F.R. (1979). Synergetics: Explorations in the Geometry of Thinking. First Published by Macmillan Publishing Co. Inc. 1975.
- Gardenfors, P. (2004). Conceptual Spaces: The Geometry of Thought. MIT Press. ISBN: 0262572192
- Gardenfors, P. (2014). The Geometry of Meaning: Semantics Based on Conceptual Spaces. The MIT Press. ISBN: 0262026783
- Gasteiger, J., Zupan, J. (1993). Neural Networks in Chemistry. Angewdante Chemie, Volume32, Issue 4, Pages 503-527 https://doi.org/10.1002/ANIE.199305031
- Georgeon, O. L., Bernard, F.J., Cordier, A.. (2015). Constructing Phenomenal Knowledge in an Unknown Noumenal Reality. BICA. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2015.12.177
- Gerber, S., Tasdizen, T., Joshi, S.C., Whitaker, R.T. (2009). On the Manifold Structure of the Space of Brain Images. MICCAI. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-04268-3_38
- Gold, M., Murray, R.F., Bennett, P.J, Sekuler, A.B. (2000). Deriving Behavioural Receptive Fields for Visually Completed Contours. Current Biology. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00523-6
- González, J.M. (2006). The Meaning and Function of Phantasia in Aristotle’s Rhetoric III.1. https://doi.org/10.1353/APA.2006.0006
- Hameroff, S., Penrose, R. (2014). Consciousness in the universe: A review of the ‘Orch OR’ theory. Physics of Life Reviews, Volume 11, Issue 1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2013.08.002.
- Haun, A., & Tononi, G. (2019). Why Does Space Feel the Way It Does? Towards a Principled Account of Spatial Experience. Entropy, 21(12), 1160.
- Hamed, N.A., Trnka, J. (2013). The Amplituhedron. ArXiv, https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1312.2007
Hegel, G.W.F. (1816/2010). Science of Logic, Volume 7 of Hegel in 7 volumes, Muirhead library of philosophy. Taylor & Francis Group. ISBN 0415606608 - Heusser, C., Fitzpatrick, C.P, Manning, J.R. (2018). Geometric models reveal behavioural and neural signatures of transforming experiences into memories. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01051-6
- Hohol, M. (2020). Foundations of Geometric Cognition. Routledge.
- Hoffman, D. D., & Prakash, C. (2014). Objects of Consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 577.
- Jantzen, B.C. (2010). No two entities without identity. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-010-9717-3
- Jauernig, A. (2021). Kantian Things in Themselves, Leibniz-Wolffian Things in Themselves, and Fictionalism. https://doi.org/10.1093/OSO/9780199695386.003.0006
- Kamitani, Y., Tong, F. (2005). Decoding the Visual and Subjective Contents of the Human Brain. Nature Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1444
- Kant, I. (1999). Critique of Pure Reason. The Cambridge Edition of the Works of Immanuel Kant.
- Kastrup, B. (2018). The Universe in Consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, Vol. 25 (5-6), pp 125-155, ISSN 355-8250
- Keil, S. (2007). Gradient Representations and the Perception of Luminosity. Vision Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.09.018
- Kent, L. (2023). Mental Gravity: Depression as Spacetime Curvature of the Self, Mind, and Brain. Entropy (Basel). doi: 10.3390/e25091275
- Köhler, W. (1970). Gestalt Psychology: An Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:142507462
- Kuipers, A.F. (2019). Nomic Truth Approximation by Empirical Progress Revisited. Nomic Truth Approximation Revisited, pp 21–39. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-98388-2_2
- Lakoff, G. (1973). Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. J. Philos. Log.. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00262952
- Lakoff, G. (2003). Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press. ISBN: 0226468011
- Langan, C.M. (2002). The Cognitive-Theoretic Model of the Universe A New Kind of Reality Theory. Mega Foundation Press. ISBN 9780971916227
- Lappin, J., Wason, T.D. (1991). The perception of geometrical structure from congruence. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781482295177-41
- Llinás, R., Ustinin, M. (2014). Frequency-pattern functional tomography of magnetoencephalography data allows new approach to the study of human brain organization. Front. Neural Circuits. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00043
- Lohmar, D. (2010). The Function of Weak Phantasy in Perception and Thinking. Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp 159–177 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2646-0_9
- Marshall, C. (2018). Never Mind the Intuitive Intellect: Applying Kant’s Categories to Noumena. Kantian Review. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136941541700036X
- Melamed, Y. (2009, 2013). Spinoza’s Metaphysics of Substance: The Substance Mode Relation as a Relation of Inherence and Predication. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, Vol. 78 (1), pp 17-82. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1933-1592.2008.00231.X
- Miller, K. (2008). Thing and Object. Acta Analytica, Volume 23, pp 69–89.https://doi.org/10.1007/S12136-008-0021-5
- Nasr, K., Viswanathan, P., Nieder, A. (2019). Number detectors spontaneously emerge in a deep neural network designed for visual object recognition. Sci Adv.10.1126/sciadv.aav7903
- Neven H, Zalcman A, Read P, Kosik KS, van der Molen T, Bouwmeester D, Bodnia E, Turin L, Koch C. (2024) Testing the Conjecture That Quantum Processes Create Conscious Experience. Entropy. 26(6):460. https://doi.org/10.3390/e26060460
- Noel, J. (1997). Interpreting Aristotle’s Phantasia and Claiming its Role Within Phronesis. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:170694052
- Noel, J. (1999). Phronesis and Phantasia: Teaching with Wisdom and Imagination. Journal of Philosophy of Education, Vol. 33 (2), pp 277–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9752.00136
- O’Gorman, N. (2005). “Aristotle’s Phantasia in the Rhetoric: Lexis, Appearance, and the Epideictic Function of Discourse. Philosophy & Rhetoric. https://doi.org/10.1353/PAR.2005.0009
- Owen, L.L.W., Chang, T., Manning, J. (2021). High-level cognition during story listening is reflected in high-order dynamic correlations in neural activity patterns. Nat Commun., Vol. 12(1). DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-25876-x
- Penrose, R. (1996). Shadows of the Mind: A Search for the Missing Science of Consciousness. Oxford University Press.
- Penrose, R. (2002). The Emperor’s New Mind: Concerning Computers, Minds, and the Laws of Physics (Popular Science) 1st Edition. Oxford University Press. ISBN: 0192861980
- Pfeiffer, C., Grivaz, P., Herbelin, B., Serino, A., Blanke, O. (2016). Visual gravity contributes to subjective first-person perspective. Neuroscience of Consciousness. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niw006
- Pierce, C.S. (1878). How to Make Our Ideas Clear. Popular Science Monthly 12, pp 286-302
- Pozzo, T., Papaxanthis, C., Stapley, P., Berthoz, A. (1998). The sensorimotor and cognitive integration of gravity. Brain Research Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00030-7
- Pribram, K. (1977). Holonomy and structure in perception. Department of Psychology, Standford, California
- Rescher, N. (2005). On the status of ‘things in themselves’ in Kant. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01064348
- Riddle J, Schooler JW. (2024) Hierarchical consciousness: the Nested Observer Windows model. Neurosci Conscious. doi: 10.1093/nc/niae010
- Romero, G. (2013). The ontology of General Relativity. api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:117804249
- Russell, Bertrand. (1918). The Philosophy of Logical Atomism. The Monist.
- Russell, Bertrand. (1927). The Analysis of Matter. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co.,/ Harcourt, Brace and Co.
- Sarris, V. (1989). Max Wertheimer on seen motion: Theory and evidence. Psychological research. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309358
- Schäfer, L. (2008). Nonempirical reality: Transcending the physical and spiritual in the order of the one. Journal of Religion&Science, Vol. 43 (2), pp 329-352. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-9744.2008.00920.X
- Schopenhauer, A. (1966). The World as Will and Representation, Vol. 1. Translated by E. F. J. Payne. Dover Publications
- Schopenhauer, A.(2020). The World as Will and Representation. Vol. 2. Translated by Norman, Judith; Welchman, Alistair; Janaway, Christopher. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1108964319.
- Segal, J. (1985). Phantasy in Everyday Life: A Psychoanalytical Approach to Understanding Ourselves. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429478253
- Selak, S. (1986). Non-Euclideanism in general relativity and cosmology. Astrophysics and
- Space Science, Vol. 127, pp 117-131. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00637767
- Sider, T. (2020). Nomic Essentialism. The Tools of Metaphysics and the Metaphysics of Science, pp 23-24. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198811565.003.0002
- Simon, J. (1972). Phenomena and Noumena: On the Use and Meaning of the Categories. Proceedings of the Third International Kant Congress, pp 521-527. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-3099-1_52
- Slowik, E. (2016). Situating Kant’s Pre-Critical Monadology: Leibnizian Ubeity, Monadic Activity, and Idealist Unity. Early Science and Medicine, Vol. 21, pp 332-349. https://doi.org/10.1163/15733823-00214P03
- Smolin, L. (2022). Views, variety and celestial spheres. ArXiv, https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2202.00594.
- Spelke, E., Dehaene, S, Hinchey, D., Izard, V., Pica, P. (2011). Geometry as a Mental Universal Construction. Space, Time and Number in the Brain, Elsevier, pp.319-332
- Sun, Y., Wen, G.. (2016). Cognitive gravitation model-based relative transformation for classification. Soft Computing, Vol.21(18), pp 5425–5441. doi:10.1007/s00500-016-2131-0
- Tononi, G. (2015). Integrated Information Theory. Scholarpedia, Vol. 10(1), 4164.
- Vallentyne, P. (2004). The Nomic Role Account of Carving Reality At the Joints. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005024818347
- Watson, J., Banks, M., Von Hofsten, C., Royden, Constance S. (1992). Gravity as a Monocular Cue for Perception of Absolute Distance and/or Absolute Size. Perception, Vol. 21(1), pp 69-76. https://doi.org/10.1068/p210069
- Weed, L. E. (2002). Kant’s Noumenon and Sunyata. Asian Philosophy, Vol. 12, pp 77-95. https://doi.org/10.1080/0955236022000043838
- Weinstein, E. (2021). “Geometric Unity.”
- Wen, G., Wei, J., Wang, J., Zhou, T., Chen, L. (2013). Cognitive gravitation model for classification on small noisy data. Neurocomputing, Vol. 118, pp 245-252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucom.2013.02.033
- Wheeler, J. A., Wojciech, H.Z. (1983). Quantum Theory and Measurement. Princeton University Press.
- Zhang, H., Rich, P.D., Lee, A.K., Sharpee, T.O. (2022). Hippocampal spatial representations exhibit a hyperbolic geometry that expands with experience. Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 26, pp131–139. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01212-4
- Zimmerman, D.W. (1982). The Universe—An Unscientific Concept. The Psychological Record, Vol. 32, pp 337–347. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394793
© 2025, Tib Roibu